000 | 01838nab a2200265 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
003 | OSt | ||
005 | 20220803091113.0 | ||
007 | cr aa aaaaaaaa | ||
008 | 220719b |||||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d | ||
100 |
_aCajic,Tatjana Seizova _949081 |
||
245 | _aMotion-Induced Scotoma/ | ||
260 |
_bsage _c2019 |
||
300 | _aVol 48, Issue 2, 2019: (115-137 p.). | ||
520 | _aWe investigated artificial scotomas created when a moving object instantaneously crossed a gap, jumping ahead and continuing its otherwise smooth motion. Gaps of up to 5.1 degrees of visual angle, presented at 18° eccentricity, either closed completely or appeared much shorter than when the same gap was crossed by two-point apparent motion, or crossed more slowly, mimicking occlusion. Prolonged exposure to motion trajectories with a gap in most cases led to further shrinking of the gap. The same gap-shrinking effect has previously been observed in touch. In both sensory modalities, it implicates facilitation among codirectional local motion detectors and motion neurons with receptive fields larger than the gap. Unlike stimuli that simply deprive a receptor surface of input, suggesting it is insentient, our motion pattern skips a section in a manner that suggests a portion of the receptor surface has been excised, and the remaining portions stitched back together. This makes it a potentially useful tool in the experimental study of plasticity in sensory maps. | ||
650 |
_aartificial scotoma, _949082 |
||
650 |
_acompletion, _949083 |
||
650 |
_afilling-in, _949084 |
||
650 |
_amotion, _949085 |
||
650 |
_aplasticity _949086 |
||
700 |
_aAdamian, Nika _949087 |
||
700 |
_aDuyck, Marianne _949088 |
||
773 | 0 |
_012374 _916462 _dSage, _tPerception _x1468-4233 |
|
856 | _uhttps://doi.org/10.1177/0301006619825769 | ||
942 |
_2ddc _cART |
||
999 |
_c12401 _d12401 |